Welcome to Francis Academic Press

Frontiers in Sport Research, 2023, 5(7); doi: 10.25236/FSR.2023.050704.

The Influence of Open-skilled Sports on Individual’s Interference Inhibition Abilities

Author(s)

Ma Chao, Yang Zhenyu, Xiao Zifan, Zhao Zhongxiang

Corresponding Author:
Ma Chao
Affiliation(s)

School of Psychology, Northwest Normal University, Lanzhou, Gansu, China, 730000

Abstract

Sports activities can be divided into open-skilled and close-skilled according to their contexts and predictabilities. Compared with closed-skilled sports, open-skilled sports are more complex and involve more cognitive demands and challenges, and individuals can have more opportunities to practice interference inhibition for such sports' attributes. Long-term interference inhibition training can optimize attention resource allocation, promote the efficient organization of neural networks, improve the neural processing efficiency of individuals in cognitive tasks, and strengthen top-down cognitive control, resulting in improved interference inhibition. Future studies can further explore the impact of open-ended skill sports on individual’s abilities to interference inhibition from the aspects of individual differences, dose effects, and special group interventions.

Keywords

sports activities; open-skilled sports; closed-skilled sports; interference inhibition

Cite This Paper

Ma Chao, Yang Zhenyu, Xiao Zifan, Zhao Zhongxiang. The Influence of Open-skilled Sports on Individual’s Interference Inhibition Abilities. Frontiers in Sport Research (2023) Vol. 5, Issue 7: 19-24. https://doi.org/10.25236/FSR.2023.050704.

References

[1] Ballester, R., Huertas, F., Molina, E., & Sanabria, D. (2018). Sport participation and vigilance in children: Influence of different sport expertise[J]. Journal of sport and health science, 7(4), 497-504.

[2] Chueh, T. Y., Huang, C. J., Hsieh, S. S., Chen, K. F., Chang, Y. K., & Hung, T. M. (2017). Sports training enhances visuo-spatial cognition regardless of open-closed typology[J]. PeerJ, 5, e3336.

[3] Erickson, K. I., Hillman, C., Stillman, C. M., Ballard, R. M., Bloodgood, B., Conroy, D. E., ... & Powell, K. E. (2019). Physical activity, cognition, and brain outcomes: a review of the 2018 physical activity guidelines[J]. Medicine and science in sports and exercise, 51(6), 1242.

[4] Gajewski, P. D., & Falkenstein, M. (2016). Physical activity and neurocognitive functioning in aging-a condensed updated review[J]. European Review of Aging and Physical Activity, 13(1), 1-7.

[5] Chang, Y. K., Tsai, C. L., Huang, C. C., Wang, C. C., & Chu, I. H. (2014). Effects of acute resistance exercise on cognition in late middle-aged adults: general or specific cognitive improvement? [J]. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 17(1), 51-55.

[6] Guiney, H., & Machado, L. (2013). Benefits of regular aerobic exercise for executive functioning in healthy populations[J]. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 20(1), 73-86.

[7] Khan, N. A., & Hillman, C. H. (2014). The relation of childhood physical activity and aerobic fitness to brain function and cognition: a review[J]. Pediatric exercise science, 26(2), 138-146.

[8] Tomporowski, P. D., Lambourne, K., & Okumura, M. S. (2011). Physical activity interventions and children's mental function: an introduction and overview[J]. Preventive medicine, 52, S3-S9.

[9] Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis[J]. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. 

[10] Bustamante, E. E., Williams, C. F., & Davis, C. L. (2016). Physical activity interventions for neurocognitive and academic performance in overweight and obese youth: a systematic review[J]. Pediatric Clinics, 63(3), 459-480.

[11] Drollette, E. S., Scudder, M. R., Raine, L. B., Moore, R. D., Saliba, B. J., Pontifex, M. B., & Hillman, C. H. (2014). Acute exercise facilitates brain function and cognition in children who need it most: an ERP study of individual differences in inhibitory control capacity[J]. Developmental cognitive neuroscience, 7, 53-64.

[12] Pindus, D. M., Drollette, E. S., Scudder, M. R., Khan, N. A., Raine, L. B., Sherar, L. B., ... & Hillman, C. H. (2016). Moderate-to-vigorous physical activity, indices of cognitive control, and academic achievement in preadolescents[J]. The Journal of pediatrics, 173, 136-142.

[13] Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions[J]. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135–168.

[14] Amatriain‐Fernández, S., Ezquerro García‐Noblejas, M., & Budde, H. (2021). Effects of chronic exercise on the inhibitory control of children and adolescents: A systematic review and meta‐analysis[J]. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 31(6), 1196-1208.

[15] Zhao Xin, Jia Lina, & Zan Xiangyi (2016). Training for Interference Control: Content, Effect, and Mechanism [J] Advances in Psychological Science, 24 (6), 900-908

[16] Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy[J]. Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 220–246.

[17] Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task[J]. Perception & Psychophysics, 16(1), 143–149.

[18] Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions[J]. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643–662.

[19] Simon, J. R., & Wolf, J. D. (1963). Choice reaction time as a function of angular stimulus-response correspondence and age[J]. Ergonomics, 6(1), 99–105.

[20] Formenti, D., Trecroci, A., Duca, M., Cavaggioni, L., D’Angelo, F., Passi, A., ... & Alberti, G. (2021). Differences in inhibitory control and motor fitness in children practicing open and closed skill sports[J]. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 1-9.

[21] Yu, C. C., Muggleton, N. G., Chen, C. Y., Ko, C. H., & Liu, S. (2021). The comparisons of inhibitory control and post-error behaviors between different types of athletes and physically inactive adults[J]. Plos one, 16(8), e0256272.

[22] Schmidt, R. A., & Wrisberg, C. A. (2008). Motor learning and performance: A situation-based learning approach[M]. Human kinetics.

[23] Gu, Q., Zou, L., Loprinzi, P. D., Quan, M., & Huang, T. (2019). Effects of open versus closed skill exercise on cognitive function: a systematic review[J]. Frontiers in psychology, 1707.

[24] Takahashi, S., & Grove, P. M. (2019). Comparison of the effects of running and badminton on executive function: A within-subjects design[J]. Plos one, 14(9), e0216842.

[25] Zhu, H., Chen, A., Guo, W., Zhu, F., & Wang, B. (2020). Which type of exercise is more beneficial for cognitive function? A meta-analysis of the effects of open-skill exercise versus closed-skill exercise among children, adults, and elderly populations[J]. Applied Sciences, 10(8), 2737.

[26] Galligan, F. E. A. (ed.). (2000). “Acquiring skill,” in Advanced PE for Edexcel[M]. Bath: Bath Press, 102–108.

[27] Brady, F. (1995). Sports skill classification, gender, and perceptual style[J]. Perceptual and motor skills, 81(2), 611-620.

[28] Di Russo, F., Bultrini, A., Brunelli, S., Delussu, A. S., Polidori, L., Taddei, F., ... & Spinelli, D. (2010). Benefits of sports participation for executive function in disabled athletes[J]. Journal of Neurotrauma, 27(12), 2309-2319.

[29] Wang, C. H., Chang, C. C., Liang, Y. M., Shih, C. M., Chiu, W. S., Tseng, P., ... & Juan, C. H. (2013). Open vs. closed skill sports and the modulation of inhibitory control[J]. PloS one, 8(2), e55773.

[30] Vestberg, T., Gustafson, R., Maurex, L., Ingvar, M., & Petrovic, P. (2012). Executive functions predict the success of top-soccer players[J]. PloS one, 7(4), e34731.